Marvin Minsky Zitate
seite 2

Marvin Lee Minsky war ein amerikanischer Forscher auf dem Gebiet der künstlichen Intelligenz . Gemeinsam mit John McCarthy, Nathaniel Rochester und Claude Shannon begründete er 1956 auf der Dartmouth Conference den Begriff der künstlichen Intelligenz. Später waren er und Seymour Papert auch Begründer des Labors für Künstliche Intelligenz am Massachusetts Institute of Technology .

Er veröffentlichte zahlreiche Texte zu diesem Fachgebiet sowie über verwandte Themen der Philosophie und machte auch einige Erfindungen. Er gilt als Erfinder des später im Konfokalmikroskop realisierten Messprinzips . Weitere Erfindungen sind mechanische Hände und andere Teile für Roboter, der Muse-Synthesizer für musikalische Variationen gemeinsam mit Edward Fredkin und die erste Logo-Schildkröte gemeinsam mit Seymour Papert. 1951 baute er mit Dean Edmonds SNARC , einen neuronalen Netzcomputer, der das Verhalten einer Maus in einem Labyrinth simulierte. Wikipedia  

✵ 9. August 1927 – 24. Januar 2016   •   Andere Namen Marvin Lee Minsky, ماروین مینسکی, Mārvins Minskis
Marvin Minsky: 65   Zitate 0   Gefällt mir

Marvin Minsky: Zitate auf Englisch

“If explaining minds seems harder than explaining songs, we should remember that sometimes enlarging problems makes them simpler!”

Music, Mind, and Meaning (1981)
Kontext: If explaining minds seems harder than explaining songs, we should remember that sometimes enlarging problems makes them simpler! The theory of the roots of equations seemed hard for centuries within its little world of real numbers, but it suddenly seemed simple once Gauss exposed the larger world of so-called complex numbers. Similarly, music should make more sense once seen through listeners' minds.

“It's harmful, when naming leads the mind to think that names alone bring meaning close.”

Marvin Minsky buch Society of Mind

Quelle: The Society of Mind (1987), Ch.2
Kontext: Questions about arts, traits, and styles of life are actually quite technical. They ask us to explain what happens among the agents of our minds. But this is a subject about which we have never learned very much... Such questions will be answered in time. But it will just prolong the wait if we keep using pseudo-explanation words like "holistic" and "gestalt." …It's harmful, when naming leads the mind to think that names alone bring meaning close.

“Looking back in vision is like recapitulation in music; both give us time, at certain points, to reconfirm or change our conceptions of the whole.”

Music, Mind, and Meaning (1981)
Kontext: How do both music and vision build things in our minds? Eye motions show us real objects; phrases show us musical objects. We "learn" a room with bodily motions; large musical sections show us musical "places." Walks and climbs move us from room to room; so do transitions between musical sections. Looking back in vision is like recapitulation in music; both give us time, at certain points, to reconfirm or change our conceptions of the whole.

“A memory should induce a state through which we see current reality as an instance of the remembered event — or equivalently, see the past as an instance of the present.”

K-Linesː A Theory of Memory (1980)
Kontext: A memory should induce a state through which we see current reality as an instance of the remembered event — or equivalently, see the past as an instance of the present.... the system can perform a computation analogous to one from the memorable past, but sensitive to present goals and circumstances.

“Just knowing that such states exist, that is, having symbols for them, is half the battle.”

Music, Mind, and Meaning (1981)
Kontext: Innate sentic detectors could help by teaching children about their own affective states. For if distinct signals arouse specific states, the child can associate those signals with those states. Just knowing that such states exist, that is, having symbols for them, is half the battle.

“Only the professional remembers the music itself, timbres, tones and textures.”

K-Linesː A Theory of Memory (1980)
Kontext: Concrete concepts are not necessarily the simplest ones. A novice best remembers "being at" a concert. The amateur remembers more of what it "sounded like." Only the professional remembers the music itself, timbres, tones and textures.

“We learn to see gases and fluids as particles, particles as waves, and waves as envelopes of growing spheres.”

Jokes and their Relation to the Cognitive Unconscious (1980)
Kontext: All intelligent persons also possess some larger-scale frame-systems whose members seemed at first impossibly different — like water with electricity, or poetry with music. Yet many such analogies — along with the knowledge of how to apply them — are among our most powerful tools of thought. They explain our ability sometimes to see one thing — or idea — as though it were another, and thus to apply knowledge and experience gathered in one domain to solve problems in another. It is thus that we transfer knowledge via the paradigms of Science. We learn to see gases and fluids as particles, particles as waves, and waves as envelopes of growing spheres.

“In today's computer science curricula … almost all their time is devoted to formal classification of syntactic language types, defeatist unsolvability theories, folklore about systems programming, and generally trivial fragments of "optimization of logic design"”

the latter often in situations where the art of heuristic programming has far outreached the special-case "theories" so grimly taught and tested — and invocations about programming style almost sure to be outmoded before the student graduates.
Turing Award Lecture "Form and Content in Computer Science" (1969) http://web.media.mit.edu/~minsky/papers/TuringLecture/TuringLecture.html, in Journal of the Association for Computing Machinery 17 (2) (April 1970)

“No bird discovers how to fly: evolution used a trillion bird-years to 'discover' that – where merely hundreds of person-years sufficed.”

"Communication with Alien Intelligence" http://web.media.mit.edu/~minsky/papers/AlienIntelligence.html, in Extraterrestrials: Science and Alien Intelligence (1985) edited by Edward Regis <!-- Cambridge University Press --> also published in Byte Magazine (April 1985)
Kontext: Speed is what distinguishes intelligence. No bird discovers how to fly: evolution used a trillion bird-years to 'discover' that – where merely hundreds of person-years sufficed.

“Clearly, the child is learning about space! …how on earth does one learn about time? Can one time fit inside another? Can two of them go side by side? In music, we find out!”

Music, Mind, and Meaning (1981)
Kontext: Of what use is musical knowledge? Here is one idea. Each child spends endless days in curious ways; we call this play. A child stacks and packs all kinds of blocks and boxes, lines them up, and knocks them down. … Clearly, the child is learning about space!... how on earth does one learn about time? Can one time fit inside another? Can two of them go side by side? In music, we find out!

“Concrete concepts are not necessarily the simplest ones.”

K-Linesː A Theory of Memory (1980)
Kontext: Concrete concepts are not necessarily the simplest ones. A novice best remembers "being at" a concert. The amateur remembers more of what it "sounded like." Only the professional remembers the music itself, timbres, tones and textures.

“For generations, scientists and philosophers have tried to explain ordinary reasoning in terms of logical principles — with virtually no success.”

Marvin Minsky buch Society of Mind

Quelle: The Society of Mind (1987), p. 187
Kontext: For generations, scientists and philosophers have tried to explain ordinary reasoning in terms of logical principles — with virtually no success. I suspect this enterprise failed because it was looking in the wrong direction: common sense works so well not because it is an approximation of logic; logic is only a small part of our great accumulation of different, useful ways to chain things together.

“Computer languages of the future will be more concerned with goals and less with procedures specified by the programmer.”

Turing Award Lecture "Form and Content in Computer Science" (1969), in Journal of the Association for Computing Machinery 17 (2) (April 1970)

“We'll show you that you can build a mind from many little parts, each mindless by itself.”

Marvin Minsky buch Society of Mind

Prologue
The Society of Mind (1987)

Ähnliche Autoren

Joseph Campbell Foto
Joseph Campbell 13
US-amerikanischer Mythenforscher, Professor und Autor
Milton Friedman Foto
Milton Friedman 6
US-amerikanischer Wirtschaftswissenschaftler und Nobelpreis…
Helen Rowland Foto
Helen Rowland 1
US-amerikanische Journalistin
Kurt Vonnegut Foto
Kurt Vonnegut 4
US-amerikanischer Schriftsteller
Isidor Isaac Rabi Foto
Isidor Isaac Rabi 1
US-amerikanischer Physiker
Allen Ginsberg Foto
Allen Ginsberg 10
US-amerikanischer Dichter
Truman Capote Foto
Truman Capote 74
US-amerikanischer Schriftsteller
Nikola Tesla Foto
Nikola Tesla 46
Erfinder und Physiker
Henry Miller Foto
Henry Miller 11
US-amerikanischer Schriftsteller und Maler
Eleanor Roosevelt Foto
Eleanor Roosevelt 83
US-amerikanische Menschenrechtsaktivistin